2016, Number 5
<< Back Next >>
Salud Mental 2016; 39 (5)
Correlación del funcionamiento ejecutivo y la potencia absoluta del EEG en niños
Galicia-Alvarado M, Flores-Ávalos B, Sánchez-Quezada A, Yáñez-Suárez Ó, Brust-Carmona H
Language: Spanish
References: 55
Page: 267-274
PDF size: 211.30 Kb.
ABSTRACT
Introduction
The resting EEG power is considered as a robust marker of brain function;
however, a few studies have associated it to a specific function.
Objective
To analyze the relationship between EEG absolute power (AP) with
performance on tasks of executive functioning in children.
Method
Correlational cross-sectional study of 30 children (8.6 ± 1 year). EEG
was performed with eyes closed and bipolar leads. With the Fourier
transform AP was calculated in the spectrum of 1.6, 30 Hz. NEUROPSI
Attention and Memory was applied and the Index of Attention and
Executive functions (IAFE) was calculated. Based on this, the AP was
compared with Kruskal-Wallis and the Z value, Spearman coefficient
for correlation with the subtests.
Results
Children with severe impairment (x = 63 ± 8 CI 95% [57.2, 68.5])
had higher delta AP (α ≤ .05) in F1F7, P301 and P402, and Z values
near 2
SD in F7T3, F3C3 and F8T4 at slow frequencies. Correlations
were significant between the IAFE and delta AP P301 (-.57), P402
(-.43) and T5O1 (-.37); with AP alpha mainly on left fronto- temporo
- parietal- occipital areas. The score in Visual Detection and Semantic
Fluency inversely related to AP alpha frequency.
Discussion and conclusion
Performance on tasks of executive functioning is different in relation
to the frontal and parietal AP delta. There is an inverse relationship
between AP delta and alpha at rest with attention and verbal fluency.
REFERENCES
Goldman-Rakic P. Cellular basis of WM. Neuron 1995;14:477-485.
Goldberg E. The executive brain: Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. Oxford: University Press; 2001.
Miyake A et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 2000;41:49-100.
Anderson V et al. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev Neuropsychol 2001;20:385-406.
Huizinga M et al. Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:2017-2036.
Lores-Lázaro J et al. Desarrollo de funciones ejecutivas, de la niñez a juventud. Anales Psicologia 2014;30:463-473.
Best J, Miller P. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev 2010;81:1641-1660.
Romine C, Reynolds C. A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: findings from a meta-analysis. Appl Neuropsyschol 2005;12:190-201.
Best J, Miller P, Jones L. Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Dev Rev 2009;29:180-200.
Michels L et al. Developmental changes of functional and directed resting- state connectivities associated with neuronal oscillations in EEG. Neuroimage 2013;81:231-242.
Lüchinger R et al. Brain state regulation during normal development : Intrinsic activity fluctuations in simultaneous EEG–fMRI. Neuroimage 2012:60:1426-1439.
Fair D et al. Atypical default network connectivity in youth with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2010:68:1084–1091.
Fair D et al. The maturing architecture of the brain’s default network. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008;105:4028-4032.
Buckner R, Vincent J. Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. NeuroImage 2007;37:1091–1096.
Laufs H et al. Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003;100:11053-11058.
Raichle M et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98:676–682.
Harmony T et al. EEG delta activity: An indicator of attention to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. Int J Psychophysiol 1996;24:161-171.
Fernández T et al. Relationship of specific EEG frequencies at specific brain areas with performance. Neuroreport 1998;9: 3681-3687.
Knyazev G. EEG delta oscillations as a correlate of basic homeostatic and motivational processes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012;36:677-695.
Klimesch W. Alpha and beta band power changes in normal and dyslexic children. Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:1186-1195.
Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Rev 1999;29:169- 195.
Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations : The inhibition – timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 2007;53:63-88.
Buzsáki G, Kaila K, Raichle M. Inhibition and brain work. Neuron 2007;56:771–783.
Knyazev G. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2007;31:377-395.
Szava S et al. High resolution quantitative EEG analysis. Brain Topogr 1994;6:211-219.
Buzsáki G. Rhythms of the brain. Oxford: Oxford University; 2006.
Matute E, Rosselli M, Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solís F. Evaluación neuropsicológica infantil. México: Manual Moderno; 2007.
Ostrosky-Solís F et al. Neuropsi atención y memoria 6 a 85 años. México: American Book Store; 2003.
Ostrosky-Solís F et al. Neuropsi attention and memory: A neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Appl Neuropsychol Adult 2007;14:156-170.
Gómez-Pérez E, Ostrosky-Solís F. Attention and memory evaluation across the life span: Heterogeneous effects of age an education. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006;58:477-494.
Harmony T, Fernadez-Bouzas A. Mapeo del EEG en el estudio de los pacientes con lesiones expansivas intracraneales. Archivos Clínica Neurológica Querétaro 1995;4:20-24.
Gonzalez Garrido A et al. Comparison of EEG abnormal activities in learning disabled, behavioral disordered and normal children. Arch Inst Nac Neurol Neurocir Mex 1993;8:67-72.
Massar S, Kenemans J, Schutter D. Resting-state EEG theta activity and risk learning: sensitivity to reward or punishment? Int J Psychophysiol 2014;91:172-177.
Harmony T et al. Correlation between EEG spectral parameters and an educational evaluation. Int J Neurosci 1990;54:147-155.
Harmony T et al. Longitudinal quantitative EEG study of children with different performances on a reading-writing test. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:426-433.
Matousek M, Rasmussen P, Gilberg C. EEG frequency analysis in children with so called minimal brain dysfunction and related disorders. Adv Biol Psychiatry 1984;15:102-108.
Clarke A et al. EEG analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Res 1998;81:19-29.
Clarke A et al. Age and sex effects in the EEG: Differences in two subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:815-826.
Snyder S, Hall J. A meta-analysis of quantitative EEG power associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder J Clin Neurophysiol 2006;23:440-455.
Chabot R, Serfontein G. Quantitative Electroencephalographic Profiles of Children with Attention Deficit Disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1996;15:951-963.
Chabot R et al. Behavioural and electrophysiological predictors of treatment response to stimulants in children with attention disorders. J Child Neurol 1999;14:343-351.
Clarke A et al. Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:1333-1341.
Seeley W et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007;27:2349-2356.
Hampson M et al. Brain Connectivity related to working memory performance. J Neurosci 2006;26:13338-13343.
Laufst H et al. Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003;100:11053-11058.
Sutton S et al. Resting cortical brain activity and social behavior in higher functioning children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005;46:211-222.
Başar E et al. Brain’s alpha, beta, gamma, delta, and theta oscillations in neuropsychiatric diseases: Proposal for biomarker strategies. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol 2013;62:19-54.
Başar E. EEG-brain dynamics: Relation between EEG and brain evoked potentials. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1980.
Brandt M, Jansen B. The relationship between prestimulus alpha amplitude and visual evoked potential amplitude. Int J Neurosci 1991;61:261-268.
Başar E et al. Spontaneous EEG theta activity controls frontal visual evoked potential amplitudes. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysio 1998;108:101-109.
Barry R, Kirkaikul S, Hodder D. EEG alpha activity and the ERP to target stimuli in an auditory oddball paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;39:39-50.
Barry R et al. Preferred EEG brain states at stimulus onset in a fixed interstimulus interval equiprobable auditory Go/NoGo task: A definitive study. Int J Psychophysiol 2014;94: 42-58.
Fassbender C et al. A lack of default network suppression is linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain Res 2009;1273:114-128.
Proal E et al. Actividad funcional cerebral en estado de reposo: redes en conexión. Rev Neurol 2011;52(Supl 1):S3-10.
Weissman D et al. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 2006;9:971-978.