2023, Number 4
<< Back Next >>
Rev Elec Psic Izt 2023; 26 (4)
Circadian rhythms on a time-place learning task
Hernández VMS, Yáñez RF, García GD, Aguilar GF; Moreno GS, Carpio RCA
Language: Spanish
References: 44
Page: 1256-1272
PDF size: 309.12 Kb.
ABSTRACT
Time-Place learning has been defined as the capability of adjust to
the spatial and temporal variability of relevants events. There is
evidence of wide variety of species that have been adjusted to the
task, such rats, cumingbirds, pigeons, ants, bees, among others.
However, there is a discrepancy related to how the human being
learn spatial and temporal contingencies. In that way, the aim was
to identify corrects, oposites and incorrects percentage responses
in a daily Time-Place Learning task with university students. The
results suggest that the participants clicked more to the correct
option in morning sessions no matter if it was morning session or
afternoon session. To get closer to the answer of whether humans
adjust to a daily TPL task, more training sessions should be
performed, the interval between sessions increased, and probe
sessions performed to assess their performance and thus, could
conclude if the human being is capable to adjust their behavior to
temporal and spatial contingecies.
REFERENCES
Biebach, H., Falk, H. y Krebs, J. (1991). The effect of constant light and phaseshifts on a learned time-place association in garden warblers (Sylvia borin) :hourglass or circadian clock? Journal of Biological Rhythyms, 6(4), 353-365.https://doi.org/10.1177/074873049100600406
Biebach, H., Gordjin, M. y Krebs, J. (1989). Time-and-place Learning by gardenwarblers, Sylvia borin. Animal Behaviour, 37(3), 353-360.https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(89)90083-3
Biebach, H., Krebs, J. y Falk, H. (1994). Time-place learning, food availability andthe exploitation of patches in garden warblers, Sylvia borin. AnimalBehaviour, 48(2), 273-284. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1241
4 . Blatter, K., Graw, P., Munch, M., Knoblauch, V., Wirz-Justice, A. y Cajochen, C.(2006). Gender and age differences in psychomotor vigilance performanceunder differential sleep pressure conditions. Behavioural Brain Research,168(2), 312-317. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.11.018
Breed, M.D., Stocker, E.M., Baumgartner, L.K. y Vargas, S.A. (2002). Time-placelearning and the ecology of recruitment in a stingless bee, Trigona amalthea(Hymenoptera, Apidae). Apidologie, 33(3), 251-258.https://doi.org/10.1051/apido:2002018
Campbell, S., Murphy, P. y Boothroyd, C. (2001). Long-term time estimation isinfluenced by circadian phase. Physiology y Behavior, 72(4), 589-593.10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00414-0
Carr, J. y Wilkie, D. (1997). Rats use an ordinal timer in a daily time-place learningtask. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,23(2), 232-247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.23.2.232
Carr, J. y Wilkie, D. (1999). Rats are reluctant to use circadian timing in a dailytime-place task. Behavioural Processes, 44(3), 287-299. 10.1016/s0376-6357(98)00036-9
Carr, J., Tan, A., Thorpe, C. y Wilkie, D. (2001). Further evidence of joint timeplacecontrol of rats' behavior: results from an 'open hopper' test.Behavioural Processes, 53, 147-153. 10.1016/S0376-6357(01)00138-3
Crystal, J. (2009). Theoretical and conceptual issues in time-place discrimination.European Journal of Neuroscience. 30, 1756-1766.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06968.x
Crystal, J. y Miller, B. (2002). Simultaneous temporal and spatial processing.Animal Learning y Behavior. 30, 53-65. 10.3758/bf03192909
D’Reaux, R., Neumann, C. y Rhymer, K. (2000). Time of day of testing andneuropsychological performance of schizophrenic patients and healthycontrols. Schizophrenia Research, 45, 157-167. 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00196-6
Deibel, S y Thorpe, C. (2013). The effects of response cost and species-typicalbehaviors on a daily time-place learning task. Learning Behavior, 41, 42 –53. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0076-4
Falk, H., Biebach, H. y Krebs, J. (1992). Learning a time-place pattern of foodavailability: a comparison between an insectivorous and a granivorousweaver species (Ploceus bicolor and Euplectes hordeaceus). BehavioralEcology and Sociobiology. 31, 9-15. http://www.jstor.org/stable/4600715
Folkard, S. y Monk, T. (1980). Circadian rhythms in human memory. British Journalof Psychology, 71, 295-307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01746.x
García-Gallardo, D. y Carpio, C. (2016). Effects of variable sequences of foodavailability on interval time-place learning by pigeons. BehaviouralProcesses, 130(1), 53-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.07.008
García-Gallardo, D., Aguilar Guevara, F., Armenta, B. y Carpio, C. (2018).Aprendizaje de tiempo y lugar: efectos de la duración del periodo dedisponibilidad y de la variabilidad o constancia de la secuencia delocaciones. Universitas Psychologica, 17(4).https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy17-4.atle
García-Gallardo, D., Aguilar, F., Armenta, B. y Carpio, C. (2015). Human strategiesfor solving a time-place learning task: the role of counting and followingverbal cues. Behavioural Processes, 113, 143-151.10.1016/j.beproc.2015.01.017
García-Gallardo, D., Aguilar, F., Moreno, S., Hernández, M. y Carpio, C. (2019).Evidence of non-circadian timing in a low response-cost daily Time-PlaceLearning task with pigeons Columba Livia. Behavioural Processes, 168, 1-12. 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103942
Gómez-Laplaza, L. y Morgan, E. (2005). Time-place learning in the cichlidangelfish, Pterophyllum scalare. Behavioural Processes, 70(2), 177-181.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.021
Kraemer, S., Danker-Hopfe, H., Dorn, H., Schmidt, A., Ehlert, I. y Herrmann, W.(2000). Time-of-day variations of indicators of attention: performance,physiologic parameters, and self-assessment of sleepiness. BiologicalPsychiatry, 48(11), 1069-1080. 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00908-2
Krebs, J. y Biebach, H. (1989). Time-place learning by garden warblers (SylviaBorin): Route or map? Ethology. 83, 248-253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1989.tb00532.x
Kuriyama, K., Uchiyama, M., Suzuki, H., Tagaya, H., Ozaki, A., Aritake, S., Kame,Y., Nishikawa, T. y Takahashi, K. (2003). Circadian fluctuation of timeperception in healthy human subjects. Neuroscience Research, 46, 23-31.10.1016/s0168-0102(03)00025-7
Means, L.W., Arolfo, M.P., Ginn, S.R., Pence, J.D., y Watson, N.P. (2000). Ratsmore readily acquire a time-of-day go no-go discrimination than a time-ofdaychoice discrimination. Behavioural Processes, 52, 11-20.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00109-1
Monk, T. y Carrier, J. (1997). Speed of mental processing in the middle of thenight. Sleep, 20(6), 399-401. 10.1093/sleep/20.6.399
Murphy, C. M., y Breed, M. D. (2008). Time-place learning in a neotropicalstingless bee, Trigona fulviventris Gurin (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Journal ofthe Kansas Entomological Society, 81(1), 73–76.https://doi.org/10.2317/JKES-704.23.1
Natale, V., Alzan, A. y Cicogna, P. (2003). Cognitive efficiency and circadiantypologies: a diurnal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(5),1089-1105. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869%2802%2900320-3
Pizzo, M. y Crystall, J. (2002). Representation of time in time-place learning.Animal Learning y Behavior. 30, 287-393.https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195963
Pizzo, M. y Crystal, J. (2004). Time-place learning in the eight-arm radial maze.Animal Learning y Behavior, 32(2), 240-255.http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196025
Pulido, M.A. y López, L. (2006). Efectos del reforzamiento demorado enProgramas definidos temporalmente. Revista mexicana de análisis de laconducta, 32(1), 39-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v32.i1.23254
Saksida, L. y Wilkie, D. (1994). Time-of-day discrimination by pigeons, columbalivia. Animal Learning y Behavior, 22(2), 143-154.https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3758/BF03199914
Schatz, B., Beugnon, G. y Lachaud, J. (1994). Time-place learning by aninvertebrate, the ant Ectatomma ruidum Roger. Animal Behaviour, 48, 236-238. https://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1232
Thorpe, C. y Wilkie, D. (2002). Unequal interval time-place learning. BehaviouralProcesses, 58(3), 157-166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00030-X
Thorpe, C. y Wilkie, D. (2005). Interval time-place learning by rats: varyingreinforcement contingencies. Behavioural Processes, 70(2), 156-167.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.06.005
Thorpe, C. y Wilkie, D. (2006). Rats' performance on an interval time-place task:Increasing sequence complexity. Learning y Behavior, 34(3), 248-254.https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192880
Thorpe, C., Hallett, D. y Wilkie, D. (2007). The role of spatial and temporalinformation in learning interval time-place tasks. Behavioural Processes,75(1), 55-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2007.01.002
Thorpe, C., Hallett, D., Murphy, M., Fitzpatrick, C. y Bakhtiar, A. (2012). Intervaltime-place learning in young children. Behavioural Processes, 91, 198-201.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2012.07.002
Trigo, E. y Martínez, H. (1994). Diseños y procedimientos de validación en lapsicología interconductual: discriminación condicional y estrategiaslongitudinales. Revista Mexicana de la Conducta, 20(1), 67-82. http://rmacmx.org/disenos-y-procedimientos-de-validacion-en-la-psicologiainterconductual-discriminacion-condicional-y-estrategias-longitudinales/
Vallar, P. y Baddeley, A. (1984). Fractionation of working memory:neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-therm store. Journalof Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 151-161.https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/S0022-5371(84)90104-X
Widman, D., Gordon, D. y Timberlake, W. (2000). Response cost and time-placediscrimination by rats in maze tasks. Animal Learning y Behavior, 28(3),298-309. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3758/BF03200263
Widman, D., Sermania, C. y Genismore, K. (2004). Evidence for time-plae learningin the Morris water maze without food restriction but with increasedresponse cost. Behavioural Processes, 67, 183-193.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.04.001
Wilkie, D. (1995). Time-Place Learning. Current Directions in PsychologicalScience, 48(3), 85-89. https://doi.org/10.1111%2F1467-8721.ep10772332
Wilkie, D. y Willson, R. (1992). Time-place learning by pigeons, Columba livia.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57(2), 145-158.https://dx.doi.org/10.1901%2Fjeab.1992.57-145
Wilkie, D., Carr, J., Galloway, J., Parker, K. y Yamamoto, A. (1997). Conditionaltime-place learning. Behavioural Processes, 40, 165 - 170.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0376-6357(97)00781-x