2021, Number 4
<< Back
Rev Elec Psic Izt 2021; 24 (4)
Is it essential to perform correctly to formulate the execution rule? preliminary data
Rodríguez MRN, Carpio RCA
Language: Spanish
References: 17
Page: 1728-1762
PDF size: 3196.51 Kb.
ABSTRACT
The prevalence of correct response percentages is considered to
obey an operational view rewarding incorrect performance. This
persistence is observed in different studies that carried out
empirical evaluations of different variables that theoretically favor
the formulation of general execution rules. In most studies, the fact
that subjects correctly stated the execution rule did not necessarily
imply correct performance in transference tests, likewise it was
observed that participants could respond correctly without
enunciating the matching criterion. This finding raises the
possibility that formulating execution rules does not necessarily
result in the occurrence of effective behavior. Consequently, this
study’s aim was to evaluate the effects of the type of effectivity
developed on the execution and the formulation of the execution
rule, using a second-order conditional discrimination procedure.
The results are discussed in the following terms: a) high
percentages of correct responses obtained during training do not
necessarily correlate with high percentages in transference tests;
b) incorrect executions observed through the training can correlate
with high percentages of correct responses in the transference
tests; and c) it is questionable to state that all “Knowing That” must
be understood as the result of “Knowing How”, which disagrees
with that supported by Ribes (2000).
REFERENCES
Carpio, C., Pacheco, V., Canales, C., Morales, G., y Rodríguez-Medina, R. (2014).Comportamiento inteligente y creativo: efectos de distintos tipos deinstrucciones. Suma Psicológica, 21(1), 36-44.
Harlow, H. (1959). Learning set and error factor theory. En S. Koch (Ed.),Psychology: a study of a science. Nueva York: McGraw-Hill.
Hayes, S.C., Brownstein, A.J., Devany, J.M., Kohlenberg, B.S., y Shelby, J. (1987).Stimulus equivalence and the symbolic control of behavior. RevistaMexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 13(3), 361-374.
Hickman, H., Moreno, D., Tena, O., Plancarte, P., Arroyo, R., y Cepeda, M.L.(2008). Analysis of the correspondence between reporting and doing in atask of matching to sample. International Journal of Hispanic Psychology,1(1), 1-15.
Martínez, H., y Tonneau, F. (2002). Conducta humana compleja. En E. Ribes(Ed.), Psicología del aprendizaje (pp. 169-190). México: Editorial El ManualModerno.
Moreno, D., Cepeda, M.L., Hickman, H., Peñalosa, E., y Ribes, E. (1991). Efectodiferencial de la conducta verbal descriptiva de tipo relacional en laadquisición y transferencia de una tarea de discriminación condicional desegundo orden en niños. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 17,81-99.
Peña-Correal, T., Ordóñez, S., Fonseca, J. y Fonseca, L. (2012). La investigaciónempírica de la función sustitutiva referencial. En M. A. Padilla y R. Pérez-Almonacid (Eds.), La función sustitutiva referencial. Análisis histórico-crítico.Avances y perspectivas. New Orleans, University Press of the South.
Plancarte, P., Hickman, H., Tena, O., Moreno, D., Cepeda, M.L., y Larios, R.M.(2000). Correspondence of verbal descriptions and matching performance ofmexican children. Behavior Development Bulletin, 9, 26-29.
Ribes, E. (2000). Instructions, rules, and abstraction: a misconstrued relation.Behavior and Philosophy, 28, 41-55.
Ribes, E. (2016). Reflexiones sobre la investigación conductual delcomportamiento humano. En M.L. Cepeda (Ed.), Comportamiento humanocomplejo. Perspectivas conductuales (pp. 155-164). Mexico: Facultad deEstudios Superiores Iztacala, U.N.A.M.
Ribes, E., y Martínez, H. (1990). Interaction of contingencies and rule instructionsin the performance of human subjects in conditional discriminations. ThePsychological Record, 40, 465-586.
Ribes, E., y Serrano, M. (2006). Efectos de tres tipos de preentrenamiento en laadquisición y transferencia de una tarea de igualación de la muestra. Actacomportamentalia, 14(2), 145-169.
Ribes, E., Cepeda, M.L., Hickman, H., Moreno, D., y Peñalosa, E. (1992). Effectsof visual descriptions on the performance of human subjects in conditionaldiscrimination. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 23-36.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. NuevaYork: Appleton-Century.
Skinner, B.F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. En B. Kleinmuntz(Ed.), Problem solving: Research, method and theory (pp. 225-257). NewYork: Wiley.
Skinner, B.F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement. A theoretical analysis. NewYork: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Thorndike, E.L. (1911). Animal intelligence. Nueva York: Macmillan.