2015, Number 2
<< Back Next >>
Arch Neurocien 2015; 20 (2)
Executive functions in male adolescents with antisocial behavior
Broche-Pérez Y, Cortés-González L
Language: Spanish
References: 52
Page: 109-115
PDF size: 102.91 Kb.
ABSTRACT
Executive functioning (EF) deficits have been implicated in antisocial behavior, however the literature on the association between neuropsychological deficits in executive functions and adolescents with antisocial behavior is limited and the findings inconsistent.
Objective: this study examines planning, mental flexibility and reversal learning in adolescents males involved in antisocial behavior and their age-matched male controls.
Material and method: the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and Hanoi Tower where used to evaluate planning, set-shifting ability and reversal learning. The sample was comprised of 38 adolescents aged between 16 and 18 years and of 38 healthy controls, matched by age and education.
Results: Adolescents with antisocial behavior showed more difficulties in planning, mental flexibility and reversal learning, as indicated by performance on the Hanoi Tower and WCST.
Conclusions: The results on Hanoi Tower and WCST suggest altered feedback processing mechanisms in adolescents males involved in antisocial behavior whereas the findings also support the notion of EF difficulties related to dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex functioning.
REFERENCES
Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. 1995.
Packwood S, Hodgetts HM, Tremblay S. A multiperspective approach to the conceptualization of executive functions. J Clin Exp Neuropsych 2011; 33(4):456-70.
Fuster JM. Executive frontal functions. Exper Brain Research 2000;133:66-70.
Mesulam MM. The human frontal lobes: transcending the default mode through contingent encoding, in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, D.T. Stuss and R.T. Knight, Editors. Oxford University Press: New York. 2002.
Broomhall L. Acquired sociopathy: a neuropsychological study of executive dysfunction in violent offenders. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 2005; 12(2):367-87.
Raaijmakers MAJ. Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: impairments in inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2008; 1(36):1097-107.
Greenûeld R, Valliant P. Moral reasoning, executive function, and personality in violent and non violent adult offenders. Psychol Rep 2007; 101:323-33.
Hoaken PNS, Allaby DB, Earle J. Executive cognitive functioning and the recognition of facial expressions of emotion in incarcerated violent offenders, non-violent offenders, and controls. Aggressive Behavior 2007; 33:412-21.
Baker S, Ireland JL. The link between dyslexic traits, executive functioning, impulsivity, and social self-esteem among an offender and non-offender sample. Law Psychiatry 2007; 30:492-503.
Miura H. Differences in frontal lobe function between violent and non violent conduct disorder in male adolescents. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2009; 63:161-6.
Roussy S, Toupin J. Behavioral inhibition deficits in juvenile psychopaths. Aggressive Behavior 2000; 26(6):413-24.
Santor DA, Ingram A, Kusumakar V. Influence of executive functioning difûculties on verbal aggression in adolescents: moderating effects of winning and losingand increasing and decreasing levels of provocation. Aggressive Behavior 2003; 29:475-88.
Willcutt EG. Validity of the executive function theor y of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry 2005;57:1336-46.
Chan RCK. Assessment of executive functions: review o finstruments and identiûcation of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsych 2008; 23:201-16.
Goel V, Pullarab SD, Grafmanc J. A computational model of frontal lobe dysfunction: working memory and the tower of Hanoi task. Cognitive Science 2001; 25:287-313.
Barceló F. Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control. J Cogn Neurosc 2006; 18:1734-48.
Barceló F, Knight RT. Both random and perseverative errors under lie WCST deûcits in prefrontal patients. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:349-56.
Lie CH. Using fmri to decompose the neural processes underlying the wisconsin card sor ting test. Neuroimage 2006;30: 1038-49.
Monchi O. Wisconsin card sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identiûed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience 2001; 21:7733-41.
Berman KF. Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin card sorting test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:1027-46.
Rogers RD. Contrasting cortical and subcortical activations produced by attentional-set shifting and reversal learning in humans. J Cogn Neurosc 2002;12:142-62.
Wang L, Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M. Neural activities during wisconsin card sorting test–meg observation. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research 2001; 12:19-31.
Nyhus E. Barceló F. The wisconsin card sorting test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a critical update. Brain Cognition 2009;71:437-51.
Grafman J Cognitive planning deûcitin patients with cerebellar atrophy. Neurol 1992;42(8):1493-6.
Mazzocco MMM, Hagerman RJ, Pennington BF. Problem solving limitations among cytogenetically expressing fragile x women. Ame J Medical Genetics 1992;43:78-86.
Roberts RJ, Hager LD, Heron C. Prefrontal cognitive processes: working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. J Exper Psych General 1994; 123(4):374-93.
Morris RG. Planning ability after frontal and temporal lobe lesions in humans:the effects of selection equivocation and working memory load. Cognitive Neuropsychology 1997;14:1007-27.
Goel V, Vartanian O. Dissociating the roles of right ventral lateral and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in generation and maintenance of hypotheses in set-shift problems. Cereb Cortex 2005;15(8):1170-7.
Newman SD. Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsych 2003;41:1668-82.
Burgess PW. The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychol 2000;38:848-63.
Feigenbaum JD, CE Polkey, RG Morris. Deficits in spatial working memory after unilateral temporal lobectomy in man. Neuropsychol 1996;34:163-76.
Muller ST, BL Piper. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J Neirosc Methods 2014;222:250-9.
Flores J, F. Ostrosky. Neuropsicología de lóbulos frontales, funciones ejecutivas y conducta humana. Rev Neuropsic Neuropsiq Neuroc 2008; 8(1):47-58.
Ardila A, F. Ostrosky. Desarrollo histórico de las funciones ejecutivas. Neuropsicología, Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias 2008; 8(1):1-21.
Bull R, KA Espy, TE Senn. A comparison of performance on the towers of London and Hanoi in young children. J Child Psych Psychiatry 2004;45(4):743-54.
Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral Sciences. 2 ed, ed. N.J. Hillsdale1988, New York: Acad Press.
Syngelaki GM. Executive functioning and risky decision making in young male offenders. Crimi Just Behavior 2009;36: 1213-25.
Blair RJR, E. Colledge, DGV Mitchell. Somatic markers and response reversal: Is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies? J Abnormal Child Psychology 2001;29:499-511.
Van-Goozen SHM. Executive functioning in children: A comparison of hospitalised ODD and ODD/ADHD children and normal controls. J Child Psychology Psychiatry 2004;45:284-92.
Paris J. Neuropsychological factors associated with borderline pathology in children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999;38: 770-4.
Zelkowitz P. Diatheses and stressors in borderline pathology of childhood: The role of neuropsychological risk and trauma. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:100-5.
Ishikawa SS. Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. J Abnorm Psychol 2001;110:423- 32.
Pineda D. Executive dysfunctions in children with attention deûcit hyperactivity disorder. Int J Neurosci 1998;96:177-96.
Diamond A. Executive functions. Ann Rev Psych 2013; 64(64):135-68.
Zelazo PD. The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols 2003;1(1):297-301.
Tamnes CK. Performance monitoring in children and adolescents: a review of developmental changes in the errorrelated negativity and brain maturation. Developmental Cognitive Neurosc 2013;(6C):1-13.
Holroyd CB. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals. Nat Neurosc 2004;7(5): 497-8.
Mars RB. Neural dynamics of error processing in medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage 2005;28(4):1007-13.
Lie CH. Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage 2006; 30(3):1038-49.
Zanolie K. Separable neural mechanisms contribute to feedback processing in a rule-learning task. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46(1):117-26.
Monchi O. Wisconsin card sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosc 2001;21:7733-41.
Tricomi E. Performance feedback drives caudate activation in a phonological learning task. J Cognitive Neurosc 2006; 18(6):1029-43.