2024, Number 3
<< Back
Rev Elec Psic Izt 2024; 27 (3)
Effects of different blackout durations and inhibition of pertinent verbalizations on insensitivity to contingencies
Díaz LRY, Carpio RCA
Language: Spanish
References: 16
Page: 1343-1359
PDF size: 554.79 Kb.
ABSTRACT
A positive relationship has been identified between the duration of a
blackout because of error and sensitivity to contingencies. The
explanation revolves on the one hand, around assuming aversive
characteristics of blackout; on the other, the emission of verbalizations
during this period is ignored. It is likely that the sensitivity observed
during this period is sponsored by the verbalization emitted during the
period and not exclusively by its duration. To evaluate this possibility,
the objective of the present study was to evaluate the effects of
different durations and the inhibition of relevant verbalizations during
the blackout period on insensitivity to contingencies in matching-tosample
tasks. 36 young adults participated, exposed to different
durations of blackout (0, 4, 8, 16's) in which relevant verbalizations
were inhibited using mathematical operations. Participants went
through six different conditions in which the matching criterion that
operated did not correspond to the one indicated at the beginning of
each condition. An execution criterion of greater than 80% correct
answers was established to transition between conditions. The results
show that the longer the duration of blackout with inhibition of relevant
verbalizations, there is greater insensitivity to contingencies. The role
of verbalizations emitted during the blackout period is discussed.
REFERENCES
Bentall, R. P., & Lowe, C. F. (1987). The role of verbal behavior in human learning:III. Instructional effects in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 47(2), 177–190.
Catania, A. C., Lowe, C. F. & Horne, P. (1990). Nonverbal behavior correlated withthe shaped verbal behavior of children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8,43-56.
Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of rule-governed behavior. Journal ofthe Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259–276.
Díaz, Y. (2020) Efectos de distintos componentes verbales consecuentes sobre elnivel de efectividad en una tarea de discriminación condicional de segundoorden [ Tesis de licenciatura, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México]Tesiunam.http://132.248.9.195/ptd2019/diciembre/0798905/Index.html
Ferster, B. y Skinner, B.F. (1957). Schedules ofreinforcement. Englewood Cliffs:Prentice-Hall.
Fox, E y Kyonka, E (2017) Searching for the variables that control human rule‐governed “insensitivity”. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 108(2),236-254.
Herrera, H. (2016).Control instruccional: historia de instrucciones, blackout yretroalimentación en una tarea de discriminación condicional en humanos(Tesis doctoral) Universidad de Guadalajara, Guadalajara, México. https://savecc.com/Tesis/2016%20-%20TESIS%20-%20Control%20instruccional.%20Historia%20de%20instrucciones%20Blackout%20y%20retroalimentacion%20-%20David.pdf
Hojo, R. (2002) Effects of instructional accuracy on a conditional discriminationtask. The Psychological Record. 52. pp 493-506.
Kaufman, A., Baron, A., y Kopp (1966) Some effects of instructions on humanoperant behavior. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1, pp. 243-250.
Lowe, C. (1979) Determinants of human behavior. En Zieler y Harzem (dirs.),Reinforcement and the organization of behavior, Vol. 1, Nueva York.
Martínez, H. y Ribes, E. (1996). Interactions of Contingencies and instructional history on conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 46, 301-318.
Martinez, H. y Tamayo, R. (2005) Interactions of contingencies, instructionalaccuracy, and instructional history in conditional discrimination. ThePsychological Record, 55(4) , pp 633-646.
Martínez, H., Ortiz, G. & González, A. (2007). Efectos diferenciales deinstrucciones y consecuencias en ejecuciones de discriminación condicionalhumana. Psicothema, 19, 14-22
Matthews, B., Shimoff, A., Catania, C., y Sagvolden, J. (1977). UninstructedHuman Responding: Sensitivity to Ratio and Interval Contingencies. Journal ofthe Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453-467
Ortiz, G. y Cruz, Y. (2011). El papel de la precisión instruccional y laretroalimentación en la ejecución y descripciones poscontacto. RevistaMexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 37(1), 69-87.
Segal, E. (1972) Induction and the provenance of operants. En Gilbert, R. yMillenson ,J. (Eds.) Reinforcement: Behavioral Analyses ( pp. 1-34) Academic.Trigo, E., Martínez, R. y Moreno, R. (1995). Rule performance and generalizationin a matching-to-sample task. The Psychological Record, 45(2), 223-241.