2024, Number 1
<< Back Next >>
Rev Elec Psic Izt 2024; 27 (1)
Delay and social discounting in college professors
Vega VCZ, Escobar GG, Nava QC
Language: Spanish
References: 47
Page: 384-407
PDF size: 393.43 Kb.
ABSTRACT
Delay discounting model helps to explain how people
consider the immediate nature of receiving low-value
rewards, rather than delayed, higher-value rewards.
Social discounting explains how people consider the
rewards that they can share with others, rather than the
rewards they obtain individually. Most of the evidence
about the application of these choice models is with
college students, however, the research on decisionmaking
in professionals such as college professors is
limited. This study aimed to describe the choice
behavior of college professors using delay and social
discounting models. Twenty-four professors from a
public university were involved in this study. The data
shows that professors tended to choose large and
delayed rewards (more self-control), as well as altruistic
choices with people considered close to them. In
contrast, selfish choices were more reflected as social
distance increased. The hyperboloid model was the
best descriptor for the delay discounting data, but the
hyperbolic model showed the best fit in social
discounting. No statistically significant relationship was
found between the area under the curve for both tasks.
The possible implications of choices with greater selfcontrol
and altruism by professors in their teaching
practice are discussed. Findings that provide internal
and external validity to this study will be highlighted.
Finally, empirical evidence is provided with a sample
not considered before with delay and social discounting.
REFERENCES
Ainslie, G. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysisof Behavior, 21, 485-489. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1974.21-485
Amlung, A., Vedelago, L., Acker, J., Balodis, I., y MacKillop, J. (2017). Steep delaydiscounting and addictive behavior: A meta-analysis of continuousassociations. Addiction, 112(1), 51–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13535
Ávila, R., y Toledo, A. C. (2014). Descuento social: una comparación por género yedad. Conductual, Revista Internacional de Interconductismo y Análisis deConducta, 2(1), 57–68. https://doi.org/10.59792/TJDY1140
Bauer, T., y Erdogan, B. (2018). Organizational Behavior. Flat World Knowledge.
Bazerman, M., y Moore, D. (2009). Judgment in managerial decision making. Nuevajersey: John Wiley y Sons, Inc.
Baum, W. M., y Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as time allocation. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 12(6), 861-874
Bell, D., Raiffa, H., y Tversky, A. (1988). Decision Making: Descriptive, Normative,and Prescriptive Interactions. Cambridge University Press.
Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., Koffarnus, M. N., y Gatchalian, K.M. (2012). Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-diseaseprocess contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities:Emerging evidence. Pharmacology y Therapeutics, 134(3), 287–297.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2012.02.004
Burnham, K. P., y Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference: Understanding AICand BIC in model selection. Sociological Methods y Research, 33(2), 261–304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124104268644
Chiavenato, I. (2009). Comportamiento Organizacional. México: Mac-Graw Hill.
Critchfield, T., y Kollins, S. (2001). Temporal discounting: basic research and theanalysis of socially important behavior. Journal of Applied BehaviorAnalysis, 34(1) 101-122.
Davis, T., y Luthans, F (1979) Leadership reexamined: a behavioral approach.Academy of Management Review, 4, 237–248
DeHart, W.B., Friedel, J.E., Lown, J.M., y Odum, A. L. (2016). The effects offinancial education on impulsive decision making. PLoS ONE, 11(7):e0159561.
Du, W., Green, L., y Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparisons of discountingdelayed and probabilistic rewards. The Psychological Record, 52, 479- 492.
Escobar, G. G., Morales, C. S., Haynes, J. M., Santoyo, C., y Mitchell, S. H. (2023).Moderate Stability among Delay, Probability, and Effort Discounting inHumans. The Psychological Record, 73(2), 149–162.https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-023-00537-1
Estévez, E., Arreola, C., y Vñaldes, A. (2014). Enfoques de enseñanza deprofesores universitarios en México. Archivos Análiticos de PolíticasEducativas, 22 (17), 1-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.14507/epaa.v22n17.2014
Franck, C. T., Koffarnus, M. N., House, L. L., y Bickel, W. K. (2015). Accuratecharacterization of delay discounting: A multiple model approach usingapproximate Bayesian model selection and a unified discounting measure.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103(1), 218–233.https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.128
García, O, Pérez, R., y Miranda, A. (2018). Los profesores-investigadoresuniversitarios y sus motivaciones para transferir conocimiento. RevistaElectrónica de Investigación Educativa, 20(3), 43-55.https://doi.org/10.24320/redie.2018.20.3.1754
González, J., Ávila, R., y Morales-Chainé, S. (2015). Descuento temporal yprobabilístico de dinero y alcohol de usuarios en tratamiento. Revista dePsicología, 24(1),1-14.
Green, L., Fry, A. F., y Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: a lifespancomparison. Psychological Science, 5(1).
Green, L., y Myerson, J. (2004). A Discounting Framework for Choice with Delayedand Probabilistic Rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769-792.
Green, L., Myerson, J., Ostaszewski, P. (1999). Amount of reward has oppositeeffects on the discounting of delayed and probabilistic outcomes. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(2), 418–427.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 13(2), 243-266.
Herrero, F. (2010). Evaluación de la utilidad en contextos de decisión deambivalencia compleja. Análisis conductual de los juicios de contingenciaen humanos. [Tesis doctoral, Universidad Complutense de Madrid].https://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/12006/1/T32533.pdf
Jiménez, H. (2021). Descuento temporal como una forma de evaluación delcomportamiento directivo. Tesis de maestría. UNAM
Jones, B., y Rachlin, H. (2006). Social Discounting. Psychological Science, 17(4),283–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01699.x
Jones, B., y Rachlin, H. (2009). Delay, probability, and social discounting in a publicgoods game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91,61–73.https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-61
Kwan, S., Eubanks, D., y Friedrich, T. (2020). Leader Decision Making Capacity: AnInformation Processing Perspective. En M, Mumford y C. Higgs (Eds.)Leader thinking skills: capacities for contemporary leadership. (1.ª ed.,pp.227-259). Routledge.
Logue, A.W., y Anderson, Y. D. (2001). Higher-Education Administrators: When theFuture Does Not Make a Difference. Psychological Science, 12(4), 276-281.
Luthans, F. (2011). Organizational Behavior. An Evidence-Based Approach. México:McGraw-Hill/Irwin
Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. InM. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin y H. Rachlin (Eds.), QuantitativeAnalyses of Behavior. Vol. 5: The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Eventson Reinforcement Value (pp. 55-73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Miranda, A., Miranda, L., Rodríguez, I., y Rodríguez, J. (2020). Deasafío de losdocentes ante la Educación 4.0. Humanidades, Tecnología y Ciencia, delInstituto Politécnico Nacional. Ejemplar 22.
Mumford, M. y Higgs, C. (2020). Leader Thinking Skills. En Aurores (eds.) Leaderthinking skills: capacities for contemporary leadership. (1.ª ed., pp. 1-14).Routledge
Museo Memoria y Tolerancia. (2024). Altruismo. Recuperado el 10 de enero del2024, dehttps://www.myt.org.mx/tolerancia_url/altruismo#:~:text=El%20altruismo%20nos%20ayuda%20a,y%20necesidades%20de%20los%20demás.
Myerson, J., Green, L., y Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area under the curve as ameasure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,76(2), 235-243. http://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235
Newell, B. R., Lagnado, D. A., y Shanks, D. R. (2015). Straight choices: Thepsychology of decision making. Routledge
Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay discounting: I’m a K, you’re a K. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 96(3), 427-439.
Odum, A. L., y Rainaud, C. P. (2003). Discounting of delayed hypothetical money,alcohol, and food. Behavioural Processes, 64, 305–313.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00145-1
Rachlin, H. (2006). Notes of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis ofBehavior, 85(3), 425–435. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2006.85-05
Rachlin, H. y Raineri, A. (1992). Irrationality, impulsiveness, and selfishness asdiscount reversal effects. In G. F. Loewenstein y J. Elder (Eds.), Choice overtime (pp. 93-118). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., y Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journalof the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233-244.
Rivera de Parada, A., Mainegra Fernández, D., y Pineda Rivas, E.E. (2024).Competencias genéricas de los profesionales universitarios priorizadas porel sector laboral: estudio referencial. COODES, 12, 1, e670. Disponible en:https://coodes.upr.edu.cu/index.php/coodes/article/view/670
Safin, V., y Rachlin, H. (2020). A ratio scale for social distance. Journal of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior, 114, 1-15.https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.614.
Sharp, C., Barr, G., Ross, D., Bihimani, R., Ha, C., y Vuchinich, R. (2012). Socialdiscounting and externalizing behavior problems in boys. Journal ofBehavioral Decision Making, 25, 239–247. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.719
Vega, V. C. Z., Méndez, F. F. A., Gómez, E. G., y Nava, Q. C. (2023). Elegir: cómohacerlo. Revista Electrónica de Psicología Iztacala, 26(4), 1245–1255.
Whetten, D., y Cameron, K. (2011). Introducción, En Autores (Eds.) Desarrollo dehabilidades directivas. p. 1-43. Pearson educación.
Yamakawa, Y., Kanal, R. Matsumura, M., y Naito, E. (2009). Social DistanceEvaluation in Human Parietal Cortex. PLoS ONE, 4(2), 1-10.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004360