<< Back
Salud Mental 2010; 33 (2)
Language: Spanish
References: 68
Page: 197-205
PDF size: 128.31 Kb.
ABSTRACT
According to recent evidence, memory can be conceptualized as a series of subsystems working together to reach the same final goal. In the present time, most authors coincide in proposing a main division or long-term memory systems in mammals. This division separates memory in two categories: declarative and not declarative, often conceptualized also as explicit and implicit memories, respectively, although the latter terms are more appropriate to describe the role of consciousness during the development of codification and recovery tasks in regard to the information of each system. Declarative memory, as indicated by its name, includes a variety of memories implicating information that can be verbalized and effectively transmitted from person to person. This type of memory is conceptualized as conformed by two distinct memory sub-categories sometimes collaborating and coinciding in the use of memory. The first of these sub-categories is semantic memory, the one including the information about concepts and precise facts, and it is frequently defined as «general knowledge». It also makes reference to conceptual information lacking from the temporal-spatial frame (this issue is detailed in the first part of this paper). The second sub-category is episodic memory, which refers to the memory for personal experienced events, or the memory used for «what?, where?, and when?», in regard to the occurrence of a given personal event. Altogether, autonoesis (the conscious concept of a personally experienced event), subjective temporal consciousness (or subjective temporal frame about when a given event occurred), and self-consciousness are considered indispensable for the definition of episodic memory. In spite of these characteristics, there is yet controversy on whether this type of memory could be observed in animals. In this regard, some researchers have demonstrated that the processes needed to define episodic memory are effectively employed by non-human primates and rodents.
In regard to the functional anatomy of episodic memory, an active role of the temporal lobe in the codification of some components of this memory is known from long time ago. Experimental studies employing hippocampal electro-physiological recordings have demonstrated that this region participates in two main components of episodic memory: i) the strong links of the subject with the environment or the spatial context, and ii) the temporal organization of the stored information. Some studies have clearly demonstrated the important participation of specific hippocampal cell areas (CA1 and CA3) for the resolution of behavioral tasks requiring episodic memory. However, clinical studies employing functional imaging have shown a considerable diversity of cortical areas as involved in the several processes of codification or recovery of episodic information, going from prefrontal medial and left ventrolateral regions to medial and lateral temporal regions, retrosplenial cortex, posterior cingulus posterior, and even the cerebellum.
Non-declarative memory systems: Non-declarative memory contains different categories: procedural memory,
priming, associative learning (classic and operant conditioning) and non-associative learning (habituation and sensitization): a) Procedural memory refers to the storage and recovery of information on motor skills, or «know how to do» distinct tasks. Although this type of memory is considered as part of those memory systems often leading to unconscious learning, the relationship between procedural memory and consciousness is more complex. This is due in part to the fact that there is no evidence of a real association between the voluntary (conscious) desire of movement and the activation of motor brain areas. In addition, it has been demonstrated that movements consciously activated to start a given task may difficult the performance of the same task. Moreover, learning of motor skills exhibits a particular characteristic known as
consolidation or
off-line stabilization. This term refers to those motor skills exhibiting an improvement in performance during the interval of two practice sessions, meaning that the performance is improved if behavior is re-analyzed after an interval of rest typically occurring along the day or during sleep periods. This supports the theory that during sleep periods there is a recapitulation of events taking place during the day, thus favoring the strengthening of neuroplastic mechanisms involved in motor learning. b)
Priming is a type of implicit memory not requiring any conscious recollection of previous experiences, and it shares some features with procedural and semantic memories. Similar to procedural memory, priming implies an increase of skills, but in this case, perceptual skills. It is also similar to semantic memory in terms of the involvement of cognitive representations of outdoor environment, and its representation is more cognitive than behavioral. The general characteristics of priming are: i) it is related with the perceptual identification of objects in general terms, including words or concepts; ii) its neuronal substrate is not depending on those brain regions needed for episodic or semantic memories; iii) it is developed early in life and its capacity remains stable all the time; iv) its activity is not related with consciousness and its function is not sufficient to recall a previous experience; v) it is relatively immune to the effects of drugs affecting the other memory systems; vi) its information is distributed in multiple representations of specific words and objects; and vii) the access to this representations is highly specific to each category of objects. Finally, among short-term memory systems, the concept of working memory proposed by Baddeley and Hitch is still accepted in our days, and is characterized by the transitory storage and administration of all kind of information useful for the achievement of a specific task. This system is based on three main components: i) a control system with limited attention capacity, also known as Executive Central Component; ii) the Phonologic Circuit, based on sound and language; and iii) the Visual-Spatial Scheme. The phonologic circuit is responsible for the transitory storage of language information based on sound and it is proponed to play an active role for learning of language. The Visual-Spatial Scheme is important for the visual-spatial representation of objects and their features, which in turn is relevant for the integration of concepts. The Executive Central Component is in charge of controlling the attention and the access to the other two subsidiary components. The knowledge on how the memory systems are functioning is and will remain in constant expansion, given the obvious contribution of recent studies on molecular biology and functional neuroimaging.
REFERENCES
Squire LR. Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2004;82:171-177.
Tulving E. Episodic memory and common sense: how far apart? Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences 2001;356:1505-1515.
Dere E, Kart-Teke E, Huston JP, De Souza-Silva MA. The case for episodic memory in animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:1206-1224.
Tulving E. How many memory systems are there? Am Psychologist 1985;40:385-398.
Tulving E, Schacter DL. Priming and human memory systems. Science 1990;247:301-306.
Tulving E. Episodic memory from mind to brain. Ann Rev Psychol 2002;53:1-25.
Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoética consciousness. Psychol Bull 1997;121:331-354.
Gilboa A. Autobiographical and episodic memory-one and the same? Evidence form prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 2004;42:1336-1349.
Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 1998;8:198-204.
Suddendorf T, Corballis MC. Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Gen Soc General Psychol Mono 1997;123:133-167.
Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS. Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica): Hiding food in the shade. Proc Biol Sci 2004;271:S387-S390.
Eacott MJ, Easton A, Zinkivskay A. Recollection in an episodic-like memory task in the rat. Learn Mem 2005;12:221-223.
Fortin NJ, Agster KL, Eichenbaum HB. Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for sequences of events. Nature Neurosci 2002;5:458-462.
Hannesson DK, Howland JG, Phillips AG. Interaction between perirhinal and medial prefrontal cortex is required for temporal order but not recognition memory for objects in rats. J Neurosci 2004;24:4596-4604.
Griffiths DP, Clayton NS. Testing episodic memory in animals: A new approach. Physiol Behav 2001;73:755-762.
Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza-Silva MA. Integrated memory for objects, places and temporal order: Evidence for episodic-like memory in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2005;84:214-221.
Shastri L. Episodic memory and cortico-hippocampal interactions. Trends Cog Sci 2002;6:162-168.
Eichenbaum H, Dudchenko PA, Wood ER, Shapiro MR, Tanila H. The hippocampus, memory and place cells: Is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 1999;23:209-226.
Eichenbaum H, Fortin NJ. Episodic memory and the hippocampus: It´s about time. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2003;12:53-57.
Eichenbaum H, Fortin NJ. Bridging the gap between brain and behaviour. Cognitive and neural mechanism of episodic memory. J Exp Anal Behav 2005;84:619-629.
Hunsaker MR, Lee B, Kesner RP. Evaluating the temporal context of episodic memory: The role of CA3 and CA1. Behav Brain Res 2008;188:310-315.
Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Rugg MD. The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory. Tren Neurosci 1997;20:213-218.
Nyberg L. Mapping episodic memory. Behav Brain Res 1998;90:107-114.
Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B. The functional neuronatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:2189-2208.
Atance CM, O´Neill DK. The emergence of episodic future thinking in humans. Learn Motiv 2005;36:126-144.
Cabeza R. Role of lateral posterior parietal regions in episodic memory retrieval: The dual attention hypothesis. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:1813-1827.
Olson IR, Berryhill M. Some surprising findings on the involvement of parietal lobe in human memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2009;91:155-165.
Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC. Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5:576-582.
Halsband U, Lange RK. Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies. J Physiol-Paris 2006;99:414-424.
Song S. Consciousness and the consolidation of motor learning. Behav Brain Res 2009;196:180-186.
Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW, Pearl DK. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potenntial). The unconscious initiation of freely voluntary act. Brain 1983;106:623-642.
Willingham DB, Salidis J, Gabrieli JDE. Direct comparison of neural systems mediating conscious and unconscious skill learning. J Neurophysiol 2002;88:1451-60.
Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC. Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5: 1-7.
Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse R, Fosse M. Sleep, learning and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing. Science 2001;294:1052-1057.
Steriade M, Timofeev I. Neuronal plasticity in thalamocortical networks during sleep and waking oscillations. Neuron 2003;37:563-576.
Eliasen JC, Souza T, Janes JN. Human brain activation accompanying explicitly directed movement sequence learning. Exp Brain Res 2001;141: 269-280.
Mars RB, Coles MG, Grol MJ, Holroyd CB, Niewenhui S et al. Neural dynamics of error processing in medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage 2005; 28:1007-1013.
Halsband U, Matsuzaka Y, Tanji J. Neuronal activity in the primate supplementary, pre-suplementary and premotor cortex during externally and internally instructed sequential movements. Neurosci Res 1994;20: 149-155.
Ferrari PF, Rozzi S, Fogassi L. Mirror neurons responding to observation of actions made with tools monkey ventral premotor cortex. J Cog Neurosci 2005;17:212-226.
Van Mier H, Tempel LW, Perlmutter JS, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Changes in brain activity during motor learning measured with PET: effects of hand of performance and practice. J Neurophysiol 1999;80:2177-2199.
Jueptner M, Weiller C. A review of differences between basal ganglia and cerebral control of movement as revealed by functional imaging studies. Brain 1998;121: 1437-1449.
Halsband U, Lange RK. Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies. J Physiol Paris 2006;99:414-424.
Shacter DL, Buckner RL. Priming and the brain. Neuron 1998;20:185-195.
Shallice T, Warrington EK. Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: a neuropsychological study. Q J Exp Psychol 1970;22:261-273.
Hebb DO. The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley; 1949.
Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: Edit Bower G.A. Academic; 1974; pp. 47-89.
Baddeley A. Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews 2003;4:829-839.
Vallar G, Baddeley AD. Fractionation of working memory. Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behavior 1984;23:151-161.
Conrad R. Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. B J Psychol 1964;55:75-84.
Conrad R, Hull AJ. Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. Br J Psychol 1964;55:429-432.
Baddeley AD. Short term memory of word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Q J Exp Psychol 1966;18:362-365.
Baddeley AD. The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long term memory for word sequences. Q J Exp Psychol 1966;18:302-309.
Baddeley AD, Wilson B. Phonological coding and short-term memory in patients without speech. J Mem Lang 1985;24:490-502.
Papagano C, Vallar G. Phonological short-term memory and the learning of novel words, the effect of phonological similarity and item length. Q J Exp Psychol 1992;44A:47-67.
Papagano C, Valentine T, Baddeley AD. Phonological short term memory and foreign language vocabulary learning. J Mem Lang 1991;30: 331-347.
Service E. Phonology, working memory and foreign language learning. Q J Exp Psychol 1992;45A:21-50.
Baddeley AD, Gathercole SE, Papagano C. The phonological loop as a lenguaje learning device. Psychol Rev 1998;105:158-173.
Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley AD et al. Pattern span: a tool for unwelding visuo-spatial memory. Neuropsychologia 1999;37:1189-1199.
Norman DA, Shallice T. En: Consciousness and self regulation. Advances in research and theory (eds.). Davidson RJ, Schwarts GE, Shapiro D. New York: Plenum Press; 1968; pp. 1-18.
Baddeley AD. The episodic buffer. A new component of working memory? Trends Cog Sci 2000;4:417-423.
Warrington EJ, Logue V, Pratt RTC. The anatomical localisation of selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia 1971;9:377-387.
Vallar G, DiBetta AM, Silveri MC. The phonological short-term store rehearsal system: patterns of impairment and neural correlates. Neuropshychologia 1997;35:795-812.
Paulesu E, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature 1993;362:342-345.
Hanley JR, Young AW, Pearson NA. Impairment of the visuospatial sketch pad. Q J Exp Pshychol 1991;43A:101-125.
Kosslyn SM et al. Visual mental imagery activates topographically organised cortex. PET investigations. J Cogn Neurosci 1993;5:263-287.
Braver TS et al. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 1997;5:49-62.
Baddeley AD. The fractionation of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1996;93:13468-13472.
Conway MA. Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia 2009;47( 11):2305-2313.